Metacognition
Definition
Metacognition can be considered a synonym for reflection in applied learning theory.
However, metacognition is a very complex phenomenon. It refers to the cognitive control and monitoring of all sorts of cognitive processes like perception, action, memory, reasoning or emoting. It is also plausible that control over such cognitive processes can be either exiplit (people are aware of it, i.e. they have "epistemic feelings" or infer things) or implicit (they don't reflect).
- Both metacognition and reflection are considered in educational psychology texts to be concerned with the process of monitoring, regulating and controlling an individuals thinking about their thinking. It is useful to consider reflection as the verb of the process of thinking about thinking whereas metacognition is the adjective used to describe the awareness of thinking. (D. Daniels, 2002)
- Metacognition = thinking about one's thinking processes. It has to do with the active monitoring and regulation of cognitive processes. (Unesco Learning without frontiers)
- Metacognition is “knowledge about executive control systems”and the “evaluation (of) cognitive states such as self appraisal and self management” (Brown, 1996).
- Metacognition is defined in the Mayer text as knowledge and awareness of one's own cognitive processes (Mayer,2003 100) ([1])
- Metacognition is "knowledge or beliefs about factors affecting one's own cognitive activities; also reflection on a monitoring of one's own cognitive processes, such as memory or comprehension" (ERIC Descriptors: 190).
- Metacognition plays an important role in student's learning strategies:
- More technically, metacognition is the ability to evaluate one's own comprehension and understanding of subject matter and use that evaluation to predict how well one might perform on a task ([2])
- This is the process where the student takes conscious control of the learning.The learner thinks about how he is thinking in a cognitive sense. For example, the learner is using metacognition if he realizes that he is having more trouble learning how to complete a fraction problem than a multiplication problem. (Monica Schott, Rich Environments for Active Learning)
- An awareness and understanding of how one thinks and uses strategies during reading and writing ([3])
Metacognition types
“"Metacognition" is often simply defined as "thinking about thinking." In actuality, defining metacognition is not that simple” (Livingston)
Flavell's typology
According to Flavell (1979, 1987) cited by Livingston (1977), metacognition consists of both metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive experiences or regulation.
(1) Metacognitive knowledge according to Livingston (1977):
- Knowledge of person variables: general knowledge about how human beings learn and process information, as well as individual knowledge of one's own learning processes
- Knowledge of task variables: knowledge about the nature of the task as well as the type of processing demands that it will place upon the individual.
- Knowledge of strategy variables: knowledge about both cognitive and metacognitive strategies, as well as conditional knowledge about when and where it is appropriate to use such strategies
(2) Metacognitive regulation according to Livingston (1977):
Metacognitive strategies are sequential processes that one uses to control cognitive activities, and to ensure that a cognitive goal (e.g., understanding a text) has been met. See self-regulation.
Types of strategies
Quoted from Blakey (1990): Metacognition is thinking about thinking, knowing "what we know" and "what we don't know." Just as an executive's job is management of an organization, a thinker's job is management of thinking. The basic metacognitive strategies are:
- Connecting new information to former knowledge.
- Selecting thinking strategies deliberately.
- Planning, monitoring, and evaluating thinking processes. (Dirkes, 1985)
Knowledge types
Paris, Cross, and Lipson (1984) describe three aspects of this self-control of strategies for learning.
- declarative knowledge: the ability to describe some thinking strategies;
- procedural knowledge: knowledge of how to use the selected strategy;
- conditional knowledge: knowledge of when to use it.
Process-oriented definition
According to NCREL:
- Developing the plan: Questions like "What knowledge will help me doing it ?", "What should I do first ?, " Why am I reading this ?"
- Implementing and maintaining the plan: Questions like "How am I doing ?", "How should I got further?", "What should I change since I am stuck ?"
- Evaluating the plan: Questions like "How well did I manage ?", "What can I learn from it ? ..
Another similar definition is put forth by Wilson (1999)
- Metacognitive Awareness relates to an individual's awareness of where they are in the learning process, their knowledge about content knowledge, personal learning strategies, and what has been done and needs to be done.
- Metacognitive Evaluation refers to judgements made regarding one's thinking capacities and limitations as these are employed in a particular situation or as self-attributes. For example, individuals could be making a judgement on the effectiveness of their thinking and/or strategy choice.
- Metacognitive Regulation occurs when individuals modify their thinking.
Levels of Metacognition
According to NCREL, Swartz and Perkins (1989) distinguish four levels of thought that are increasingly metacognitive:
- Tacit Use. The individual does a kind of thinking--say decision making--without thinking about it.
- Aware Use. The individual does that kind of thinking conscious that and when he or she is doing so.
- Strategic Use. The individual organizes his or her thinking by way of particular conscious strategies that enhance its efficacy.
- Reflective Use. The indidvidual reflects upon his or her thinking before and after--or even in the middle of--the process, pondering how to proceed and how to improve." (p.52)
Metacognition vs. cognition
Metacognition is defined as "thinking about thinking" and therefore not the same as an ordinary cognitive strategy or learning strategy, even if it is not always easy to separate them conceptually.
“Cognitive strategies are used to help an individual achieve a particular goal (e.g., understanding a text) while metacognitive strategies are used to ensure that the goal has been reached (e.g., quizzing oneself to evaluate one's understanding of that text). Metacognitive experiences usually precede or follow a cognitive activity. They often occur when cognitions fail, such as the recognition that one did not understand what one just read.” (Livingston, 1977).
A definition by an example
(in french)
Partons d'un exemple pour tenter de mieux comprendre la notion de stratégie métacognitive. L'élève qui prend des notes lors d'un cours oral va mettre en oeuvre des stratégies cognitives qui lui permettront de transcrire le message oral sous une forme synthétique en veillant à dégager les idées principales, les liens entre ces idées, la structure d'ensemble du discours...
A l'issue de la prise de notes interviendra, pour certains élèves, ce qu'on appelle un jugement métacognitif qui consiste à évaluer si les notes dont on dispose sont suffisantes pour préparer l'examen. Suite à un jugement négatif à ce niveau, va se mettre en place une phase de régulation métacognitive qui aboutira à identifier différentes stratégies cognitives susceptibles d'améliorer l'état des notes: revoir ses notes pour en améliorer la structuration, compléter ses notes à partir des notes d'un condisciple ou d'un enregistrement sonore du discours du professeur, consulter des ouvrages sur le sujet.
Comme l'illustre cet exemple, l'idée de "méta" qui accompagne le qualificatif cognitif fait référence au fait que les stratégies cognitives vont conduire à porter un jugement ou à réguler l'application des stratégies cognitives. Si on accepte ce rôle de contrôle continu des processus cognitifs joué par les stratégies métacognitives, on comprend aisément pourquoi on accorde aujourd'hui autant d'intérêt à ce type de stratégies.(Depover & et al.)
Can metacognition be taught ?
Today, most learning theorists believe that when otherwise similar, students with better metacognitive abilities are likely to be better learners. Therefore there is a logical interest for instruction of metacognitive strategies. Some studies show that instruction in metacognitive strategies induced increases in learning (Scruggs, 1985).
"While there are several approaches to metacognitive instruction, the most effective involve providing the learner with both knowledge of cognitive processes and strategies (to be used as metacognitive knowledge), and experience or practice in using both cognitive and metacognitive strategies and evaluating the outcomes of their efforts (develops metacognitive regulation). Simply providing knowledge without experience or vice versa does not seem to be sufficient for the development of metacognitive control" (Livingston, 1996).
Merlo et al. (2007) use the overall pedagogical design of Schneider & Pressley (2007) and Brown et al., (1983) metacognitive model for the exercise. It is structured like this (
- an introduction in which the student is taught explicitly about a strategy and its possible applications.
- interactive exercises, or practical training requiring the student to use the skills he has learned so the procedures are more likely to be internalized and the student is aware of the practical use of a strategy. The items of each exercise are:
- a task consisting of a set of instructions which the student uses to plan the cognitive actions he needs to carry out in order to achieve a set aim;
- a plan of action providing a break down of the process, specifying the individual cognitive actions for achieving the aim of the task;
- a training area consisting of multimedia content for training;
- an evaluation area in which the student uses the strategy on his own, supported by strategic questions reminding him of the cognitive actions to put into effect;
- a self-monitoring area in which the student monitors the overall application of the strategy and describes any difficulties he encounters..
- final monitoring, consisting of a set of questions for the student’s self-evaluation of possible changes perceived as a result of the strategy training.
DSchneider (from this own personal experience) would add that exposure to situations that require metacognition must be repeated.
- See Cognitive Strategy Instruction for an instructional design model.
- See the learning strategy article for a deeper discussion on what cognitive strategies could/should be taught.
Assessing metacognition
If metacognition is to be taught then it follows that an assessment of metacognitive activity will need to be performed.
At CRESST (Center for Research on Evaluation, Standard, and Student Testing at UCLA) publishes handbooks for creating assessment materials to assess performance in a range of learning types. Metacognition is one of the learning types defined in their proposed CRESST learning model.
Two approaches are used to gather information on metacognitive processes. For domain-dependent metacognition think aloud protocols to reveal insights into thought processes For domain-independent metacognition information is gathered through questionnaires and self-reporting.
...(more assessment models needed)
Tools
- Any sort of web-technology where students and teachers can write can do to some extent, e.g. a webserver, a blog or a wiki, a C3MS, a learning e-portfolio, etc.
- See also CSCL and collaborative learning.
Links
Bibliography
- Ausubel, D. P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal material. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 267-272.
- Baker, E.L. , O'Neil, H.F., & Klien, D.C.D. (1998), A Cognitive Demands Analysis of Innovative Technologies, CSE Technical Report 454, National Center for Research on Evaluation, Standard, and Student Testing (CRESST), UCLA.
- Biggs, John B. (1987). Student approaches to learning and studying. Hawthorne,Victoria, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research. ED 308 201.
- Blakey, E., & Spence, S. (1990). Developing metacognition. ERIC Digest [4]
- Brown, A. L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Brown, A.L. (1987). Knowing when, where, and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. Advances in Instructional Psychology, 1, 77-165.
- Brown, A.L., Brandsford, J.D., Ferrara, R.A., & Campione, J.C. (1983). Learning, remembering and understanding. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognitive development (Vol. 3, pp. 77-166). NY: John Wiley.
- Bräuer, G (2000): Schreiben als reflexive Praxis. Tagebuch, Arbeitsjournal, Portfolio. Freiburg i.Br.
- Böttger, Magdalena and Martin Röll, Weblog Publishing As Support for Exploratory Learning On The World Wide Webm, Cognition and Exploratory Learning in Digital Age (CELDA 2004), Lisbon, Portugal, November 2004. PDF
- Dennis Daniels (2002). Metacognition and Reflection, Page written in the context of teacher preparation coursework. HTML, retrieved 16:51, 3 July 2006 (MEST).
- Depover Christian, Bruno De Lièvre, Jean-Jacques Quintin, Filippo Porco et Cédric Floquet. Quelques concepts clés issus du modèle cognitiviste, dans Les modèles d'enseignement et d'apprentissage.
- Dickinson, Guy (2003). Weblogs - can they accelerate expertise ?, Ultralab MA dissertation in Education. PDF
- Dirkes, M. Ann. (1985, November). "Metacognition: Students in charge of their thinking." Roeper Review, 8(2), 96-100. EJ 329 760.
- Dirkes, M. Ann. (1988, December). Self-directed thinking in the curriculum. Roeper Review, 11(2), 92-94. EJ 387 276.
- Downes, S. (2004): Educational Blogging, in: EDUCAUSE Review, vol 39, no. 5 (September/October 2004): 14-26. PDF/HTML.
- Efimova, L. & Fiedler, S. (2004): Learning Webs: Learning in Weblog Networks. HTML
- Fiedler, S. (2003): Personal webpublishing as a reflective conversational tool for self-organized learning, in: Burg, Th. N. (ed.), Blogtalks, Vienna, p. 190-216.
- Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. New York: D.Van Nostrand.
- Flavell, J. H. (1971). First discussant's comments: What is memory development the development of? Human Development, 14, 272-278.
- Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp.231-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
- Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906 - 911.
- Flavell, J. H. (1987). Speculations about the nature and development of metacognition. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, Motivation and Understanding (pp. 21-29). Hillside, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Flavell, J.H. (1981). Cognitive monitoring. In W. P. Dickson (Ed.), Children's oral communication skills (pp.35 - 60). New York: Academic Press.
- Garner, R, & Alexander, P.A. (1989). Metacognition: Anwered and Unanswered questions. Educational Psychologist, 24, 143-158.
- Guterman, E. (2003). Integrating written metacognitive awareness guidance as a ‘psychological tool’ to improve student performance. Journal for Research on Learning and Instruction,13, 633- 651.
- Heller, Mary F. (1986, February). "How do you know what you know? Metacognitive modeling in the content areas." Journal of Reading, 29, 415-421. EJ 329 408.
- Hollingworth, Rowan W. and Catherine McLoughlin (2001), Developing science students' metacognitive problem solving skills online, Australian Journal of Educational Technology 2001, 17(1), 50-63 HTML
- Kunz Patrick, Ross Dewstow and Peter Moodie (2003), A Generic Tool To Set Up Metacognitive Journals And Their Serendipitous Use, In G.Crisp, D.Thiele, I.Scholten, S.Barker and J.Baron (Eds), Interact, Integrate, Impact: Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Australasian Society for Computers in Learning in Tertiary Education. Adelaide, 7-10 December 2003. PDF, retrieved 16:51, 3 July 2006 (MEST).
- Livingston, Jennifer A. (1977), Metacognition: An Overview HTML (retrieved 12:53, 24 May 2006 (MEST)).
- MacAllister, K., Winne, P. H., Nesbit, J., Jamieson-Noel, D., Zhou, M., & Bennet, N. (2005). Tools for investigating selfregulated learning: An overview. In D. Jamieson-Noel (Organizer), New Tools, Approaches and Issues in Researching Self- Regulated Learning in Authentic Settings. Washington D.C., American Psychological Association.
- Mayer, R. (2003). Learning and Instruction. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.
- Mayer, R.E.(2001).Cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational aspects of problem solving. In Hartman,H.J. Editor (Eds.),Metacognition in learning and instruction (87-103). Dordrecht:Kluwer Academic Publishers
- Merlo, G., Seta, L., Ottaviano, S., Chifari, A., Chiazzese, G., Allegra, M. & Todaro, G. (2007). Guiding students to acquire strategies for Web learning through Gym2Learn. In T. Bastiaens & S. Carliner (Eds.), Proceedings of World Conference on E-Learning in Corporate, Government, Healthcare, and Higher Education 2007 (pp. 7260-7266). Chesapeake, VA: AACE. Abstract/PDF (Access restricted)
- Mittlefehldt, Sarah and Tina Grotzer (2003). Using Metacognition to Facilitate the Transfer of Causal Models in Learning Density and Pressure, Harvard University, Presented at the National Association of Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Conference Philadelphia, PA, March 23- 26, 2003 PDF retrieved 16:51, 3 July 2006 (MEST).
- NCREL, Strategic Teaching and Reading Project Guidebook. (1995, rev. ed.).
- Osborne, J. (2001). Assessing metacognition in the classroom: the assessment of cognition monitoring effectiveness. Unpublished manuscript, the Department of Educational Psychology, University of Oklahoma.
- Palinscar, A. S.; Ogle, D. S.; Jones, B. F.; Carr, E. G.; & Ransom, K. (1986). Teaching reading as thinking. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
- Paris, S.G, Cross, D.R., & Lipson, M.Y. (1984, December). Informed strategies for learning: A program to improve children's reading awareness and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(6), 1239-1252.
- Pullich, Leif, Usage of weblogs in Festum - a distance education program for teachers, FernUniversitaet in Hagen. PDF
- Puntambekar, S. & Stylianou, A. (2003). Designing metacognitive support for learning from hypertext: What factors come into play? In U. Hoppe, F. Verdejo, J. Kay. (Ed.), Artificial Intelligence in Education: Shaping the future of learning through intelligent technologies. Amsterdam: IOS Press.
- Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1989). Memory Development Between 2 and 20. in Smith, D., Charles, F. (2000). Metacognitive Miscalibration and Underachievement. Allied Academies National Conference Proceedings, Myrtle Beach, South Carolina. NY: Springer-Verlag.
- Scruggs, Thomas E.; Mastropieri, M. A.; Monson, J.; & Jorgenson, C. (1985, Fall). "Maximizing what gifted students can learn: Recent findings of learning strategy research." Gifted Child Quarterly, 29(4), 181-185. EJ 333 116.
- Swartz, R.J., & Perkins, D.N. (1989). Teaching thinking: Issues and approaches. Pacific Grove, CA: Midwest Publications.
- Thesaurus of ERIC Descriptors (1995), Houston.
- Wilson, J. Defining Metacognition: A step towards recognising metacognition as a worthwhile part of the curriculum. Paper presented at the AARE Conference, Melbourne, 1999.
- Winne, P. H., Nesbit, J. C., Kumar, V., Hadwin, A. F. (2006). Supporting self-regulated learning with gStudy software: The Learning Kit project. Technology, Instruction, Cognition and Learning, 3, 105-113.
- Zachary, W., & Le Mentec, J. C. (2000). Incorporating metacognitive capabilities in synthetic cognition. Proceedings of The Ninth Conference on Computer Generated Forces, Orlando, 513-521.